Skip to main content

Amygdala activation to threat under attentional load in individuals with anxiety disorder

Abstract

Background

Previous studies in healthy subjects have shown that strong attentional distraction prevents the amygdala from responding to threat stimuli. Here, we investigated the effects of attentional load on amygdala activation to threat-related stimuli in individuals suffering from an anxiety disorder.

Methods

During functional magnetic resonance imaging, spider-phobicand healthy control subjects were presented with phobia-related and neutral stimuli while performing a distraction task with varying perceptual load (high vs low).

Results

Our data revealed a pattern of simultaneously increased amygdala and visual cortical activation to threat vs neutral pictures in phobic individuals, compared with controls, occurring regardless of attentional load.

Conclusions

These results suggest that, in contrast to studies in healthy subjects, amygdala activation to clinically relevant threat stimuli is more resistant to attentional load.

Background

In accordance with theories suggesting a critical function of the amygdala in the processing of threat signals and the mediation of fear responses [1, 2], several studies found increased amygdala activation to threatening vs neutral stimuli in individuals with anxiety disorders [3–8] and in healthy subjects [9–14]. Furthermore, there are strong theoretical accounts proposing an automatic response of the amygdala to threat signals even when target stimuli are presented during attentional distraction [1, 2, 14]. Whereas some studies indeed suggest an automaticity of amygdala activation to threat-related stimuli under conditions of attentional distraction [9, 13, 14], several recent studies in healthy subjects, however, indicated a complete inhibition of differential activation to threat vs neutral stimuli within the amygdala, given sufficiently strong perceptual load by a main task [15–18]. Thus, it seems that, at least in healthy subjects, automatic activation of the amygdala to emotional stimuli does not occur when demanding cognitive tasks exhaust the available processing resources.

Bishop et al., for example, used a perceptual load task, while subjects were exposed to fearful and neutral faces. Perceptual load was induced by varying the number of task-relevant items [19, 20] within a letter string presented along with the facial expression. When perceptual identification was easy (low load), elevated state anxiety was associated with a heightened response to threat distractors in the amygdala and superior temporal sulcus, whereas individuals scoring high in trait anxiety showed a reduced prefrontal response to these stimuli. The latter finding was interpreted as weakened recruitment of control mechanisms when confronted with salient distractors. This finding is in accordance with theories assuming an imbalance between the stimulus-driven processing of salient threat-related stimuli, associated with automatic orienting, and goal-directed attentional control (for example, [21]). This would lead to a relatively stronger role of the posterior attentional systems in the brain involved in bottom-up attention as compared to the more anterior top-down control system [22]. However, in the study of Bishop et al., neither high-anxious nor low-anxious subjects showed an increased amygdala response to threat distractors when perceptual identification was more attention demanding (high perceptual load). Furthermore, high attentional load in previous studies also prevented differential activation to threat vs neutral stimuli in areas of the extrastriate visual cortex, suggesting the absence of differential processing of threat and neutral stimuli also in areas beyond the amygdala [15, 17, 23].

Thus, in line with a recent model of selective attention [19, 20], processing of task-unrelated stimuli is prevented when task-related demands exhaust perceptual capacity limits. Even though this model does not predict that also the processing of salient emotional stimuli is impaired by high perceptual load [24], it has been extended to the domain of threat processing [15, 23]. Furthermore, based on the findings of Bishop et al., effects of subjects' anxiety on the neural processing of threat-related stimuli seem to appear only during relatively low-load tasks. Thus, high load should prevent neural responses to threat and also the attentional processing of these stimuli, that is automatic orienting [22]. This position is in contrast to cognitive models of anxiety [25] predicting a mandatory processing of threat stimuli in anxious subjects or models predicting that anxiety increases the processing of threat-related signals under high demands on the central executive [21].

Even though it has been shown that high perceptual load prevents the processing of threat stimuli in anxious healthy subjects, it is unknown whether similar findings will be observed in individuals suffering from an anxiety disorder. Automatic processing of disorder-related stimuli seems to be a main feature of anxiety disorders and this might be represented in attention-independent activation of the amygdala [1, 2, 8]. An example is specific phobia, which is among the most common anxiety disorders [26]. Neuroimaging research implicates the amygdala in the processing of phobia-related stimuli, specifically in the initial detection of such stimuli and perhaps in the lowering of thresholds for the induction of rapid fear responses, rather than in the sustained processing of phobia-relevant information [8, 27, 28]. For example, activation of the amygdala in spider-phobic subjects has been demonstrated regardless of whether attention was focused on the stimuli or distracted by an unrelated foreground task [8], supporting the hypothesis that the amygdala is automatically activated by phobogenic stimuli [1, 2, 29]. Furthermore, this attention independent response in the amygdala was associated with increased activation in the extrastriate visual cortex [8], which is typically coactivated with the amygdala in spider phobia in response to phobia-related stimuli (for example, [3, 30–32]).

A recent study with spider-phobic subjects reported attention-dependent activation of the amygdala to spider pictures [33]. However, in this study the number of phobia-related stimuli and attention focus were confounded, making a clear interpretation of the results difficult. Thus, the findings might even be interpreted to support the hypothesis of automatic amygdala activation to task-irrelevant (background) spider pictures. To date, there has been no functional imaging study that employed a parametric variation of attentional load in individuals with specific phobia or any other anxiety disorder.

In the present study, we used event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to explore the question whether amygdala activation to phobia-relevant stimuli is modulated by a parametric variation of attentional distraction in patients with specific phobia. We used a perceptual load task that has been previously shown to inhibit amygdala activation to threat-related stimuli in high-anxious healthy subjects [15]. An absence of attentional modulation of amygdala activation in the present experiment would indicate a role of the amygdala in threat processing even under high attentional load in individuals with anxiety disorder. Additionally, we examined the neural activation in the visual cortex and several brain areas proposed to be involved in the processing of threat-related stimuli.

The results show a pattern of simultaneously increased amygdala and visual cortical activation to threat vs neutral pictures in phobic individuals, compared with controls, occurring regardless of attentional load. These findings suggest that amygdala activation to clinically relevant threat stimuli is resistant to attentional load.

Methods

Subjects

A total of 17 spider-phobic (mean age = 25.2, SD = 4.9) and 16 control subjects (mean age = 26.6, SD = 9.2) participated in the study. Participants were right handed female university students with normal or corrected-to-normal vision who provided written informed consent to volunteer in the study. The ethics committee of the University of Jena approved all experimental procedures. All phobic subjects fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for spider phobia according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; [34]) as assessed by a structured clinical interview [35]. According to this interview, spider-phobic subjects had no additional psychopathological disorders. In addition, spider-phobic subjects, but not controls, showed high scores on a spider phobia questionnaire ([36]; mean = 23.4, SD = 2.3 vs mean = 2.8, SD = 1.6; t = 29.55, P < 0.001). There was no difference in trait or state anxiety scores between groups ([37]; trait: mean phobics = 36.94, SD = 11.02, mean controls = 41.93, SD = 7.40; t = 1.43, P > 0.05; state: mean phobics = 38.27, SD = 10.42, mean controls = 34.45, SD = 4.45; t = 1.27, P > 0.05). Further demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics

Stimuli and tasks

Subjects were exposed to 48 different pictures of spiders and 48 different pictures of mushrooms while performing a letter search task (adapted from [15]). The spider pictures represented the disorder-related stimuli. We used mushrooms as control stimuli, mainly for reasons of comparability with several previous studies (for example, [8, 12, 38, 39]). A string of six letters written in red ink was superimposed onto the task-irrelevant spider or mushroom picture, respectively. In half of the trials (high perceptual load), the string comprised a single target letter (N or X) and five non-target letters (H, K, M, W, Z), which were arranged in random order. In the other half of the trials (low load), the letter string comprised either six Xs or six Ns. The task was to decide by button press whether the letter string contained an 'X' or an 'N'. The low-load and high-load conditions were arranged in blocks of four trials (see [15]). In total, there were 24 blocks of 4 trials each. Load was varied across blocks and picture category was varied within blocks. The stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was 4.5 sec allowing improved sampling of the BOLD response due to jittering between SOA relative to the repetition time (TR), thus representing an effective interval for event-related designs. The pictures were randomized across and within blocks with the restriction that two mushroom and two spider pictures were shown within each block. The stimuli (including the letter strings) were presented for 200 ms in random order with a resulting interstimulus interval of 4300 ms. Figure 1 shows an example of two trials. The overall picture size was 15 × 20° visual angle, with the stimuli subtending approximately 11.5 × 11.5°. After the fMRI session, participants rated the pictures using a nine-point Likert scale to assess valence (1 = 'very pleasant' to 9 = 'very unpleasant') and arousal (1 = 'not arousing' to 9 = 'very arousing'). Behavioral data were analyzed by repeated measures analysis of variance using SPSS (V. 17; SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA) with subsequent post hoc t tests (Bonferroni corrected). For analysis of performance data, one control subject had to be excluded due to technical problems during the registration of button presses.

Figure 1
figure 1

Example of the design. Shown are two trials of a low load mini block. Each load block consisted of four different randomly selected pictures. Load was varied across blocks and picture category was varied within blocks.

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

A run of 294 volumes (40 axial slices per volume, thickness = 3 mm, in plane resolution = 3 × 3 mm) was acquired (3 T; 'Tim Trio', Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a T2*-weighted echo planar sequence (echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, matrix = 64 × 64, field of view (FOV) = 192 mm, TR = 2.9 s). Additionally, a T1-weighted anatomical volume was recorded (192 slices, echo time (TE) = 6 ms, matrix = 256 × 256, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm). Preprocessing and analysis of the functional data were performed using the software Brain Voyager QX (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, The Netherlands). All volumes were realigned to the first, corrected for slice time errors, and spatially (8 mm full-width half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel) as well as temporally (high pass filter: cut-off = 0.006 Hz) smoothed. Furthermore, data preprocessing included removal of linear trends and of the mean. Anatomical and functional images were coregistered and normalized to the Talairach space. Statistical analysis was performed by multiple linear regression of the signal time course at each voxel. The expected blood oxygen level-dependent signal change for each predictor was modeled by a hemodynamic response function (based on a two-gamma-function model, which models rise and undershoot of the BOLD response, as implemented in Brain Voyager). Predictors of non-interest were the six movement parameters. The four predictors of interest were the spider pictures/low load, spider pictures/high load, mushroom pictures/low load, and mushroom pictures/high load. Statistical comparisons were conducted using a mixed-effect analysis. In the first step, voxelwise statistical maps were generated and predictor estimates (β weights) were computed for each individual. In the second step, contrasts of predictor estimates were analyzed across subjects with repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Statistical parametric maps resulting from the voxelwise analysis were considered significant for statistical values that survived a cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons. Voxel-level threshold was initially set to P <0.005 (uncorrected) to strike a balance between type I and type II errors. Thresholded maps were then submitted to a region of interest (ROI)-specific or whole brain-specific correction criterion, which was based on the estimate of the map's spatial smoothness and on an iterative procedure (Monte Carlo simulation) used to estimate cluster-level false-positive rates. After 1,000 iterations, the minimum cluster size threshold that yielded a cluster-level false-positive rate of 5% was applied to the statistical maps (11 voxels for whole brain analysis). According to our previous studies [7, 8, 12, 13, 18, 32], the following anatomical ROIs were defined a priori using Talairach daemon software [40, 41]: amygdala, insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), and fusiform gyrus, with the latter region consistently shown to be involved in the visual processing of spider pictures in spider-phobic subjects (for example, [8]). Statistical data are only shown for significantly activated voxels.

Results

Performance data

For accuracy (Table 2), a main effect of load was found (F(1,30) = 70.8, P <0.0001) due to decreased accuracy during the high-load condition. For reaction times (Table 2), a main effect of load (F(1,30) = 201.6, P <0.0001), due to increased reaction times during the high-load condition, and an interaction of Task × picture category (F(1,30) = 6.5, P <0.05), due to increased reaction times to spiders vs mushrooms during low but not high load, were found.

Table 2 Behavioral data

Rating data

Post-scanning arousal and valence ratings (Table 2) showed a main effect of group (F(1,30) = 101.3, P <0.0001; F(1,30) = 11.3, P <0.005), picture category (F(1,30) = 324.9, F(1,30) = 102.4, both P <0.0001), and an interaction of group × picture category (F(1,30) = 116.0, F(1,30) = 51.6, both P <0.0001). Post hoc analysis using t tests (Bonferroni corrected) revealed that phobic subjects rated spiders, but not mushrooms, as more arousing and unpleasant than control subjects (arousal: t = 10.7, P <0.001 (spiders), t = 0.3; P >0.05 (mushrooms); t = 10.9, P <0.001 (spiders), t = -2.0; P >0.05 (mushrooms)).

fMRI analysis

Amygdala ROI

For both the right and left amygdala, there was only a main effect of load (left: (x, y, z) = -25, -8, -12; F[1, 31] = 45.09; right: (x, y, z) = 27, -10, -11; F[1, 31] = 55.54; both P < 0.05, corrected; cluster size: left: 2771 mm3; right: 2571 mm3) and an interaction of group × picture category (left: (x, y, z) = -27, -1, -17; F[1, 31] = 10.15; right: (x, y, z) = 23, -1, -11; F[1, 31] = 11.54; both P < 0.05, corrected; cluster size: left: 108 mm3; right: 116 mm3). Thus, our data did not reveal an interaction of group × picture category × load. The main effect of load resulted from decreased amygdala activation across pictures and groups under high vs low load. The interaction of group by picture category was due to increased activation to spider versus neutral pictures in phobic subjects, as compared to healthy controls. However, as also indicated in Figure 2, the increased activation to threat vs neutral pictures in phobic subjects was independent of perceptual load.

Figure 2
figure 2

Amygdala responses to spider vs mushroom pictures. Increased activation in the right and left amygdala in phobic subjects was found regardless of perceptual load. Statistical parametric maps are overlaid on a T1 scan (radiological convention: left = right; y = -1). The plot shows the difference of parameter estimates (spider vs mushroom; mean and standard error) for the maximally activated voxel in the left amygdala.

Other ROIs and whole brain analysis

As indicated in Table 3, an effect of load was also evident in several other brain regions. While regions such as the dorsal ACC and other areas in the frontal and parietal cortex implicated in dealing with task difficulty showed increased activation during high load, other areas such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which is typically deactivated during demanding tasks, as well as areas in the visual cortex showed a decreased activation under high vs low load.

Table 3 Significant brain activation

Furthermore, there were main effects of picture category in the superior temporal gyrus and the visual cortex due to decreased (superior temporal gyrus (STG)) and increased (visual cortex) activation to spiders vs mushrooms across subjects and tasks (see Table 3). Most importantly, there was an interaction of group × picture specifically in the left fusiform gyrus (see Table 2 and Figure 2). The interaction of group by picture category was due to increased activation to spiders versus neutral pictures in phobic subjects, as compared to healthy controls. As also indicated in Figure 3, the increased activation to threat vs neutral pictures in phobic subjects was independent of perceptual load, comparable with the profile of activation in the amygdala. There were no further significant main effects or interactions.

Figure 3
figure 3

Activation to spider vs mushroom pictures in the extrastriate visual cortex. Increased activation in the left fusiform gyrus in phobic subjects was found regardless of perceptual load. Statistical parametric maps are overlaid on a T1 scan (radiological convention: left = right; y = -55). The plot shows the difference of parameter estimates (spider vs mushroom; mean and standard error) for the maximally activated voxel.

Discussion

The present study provides evidence for a critical involvement of the amygdala in threat processing under attentional load in subjects suffering from an anxiety disorder. Thus, amygdala activation to disorder-related vs neutral stimuli was observed regardless of attentional load. A similar finding was evident for the left fusiform gyrus.

This finding contrasts with prior studies in healthy subjects [15–18], especially with a study where the same task resulted in a strong modulation of amygdala activation to fearful vs neutral faces, with the differential amygdala activation completely inhibited under the high-load condition [15]. The study of Bishop et al. found that, during a low-load condition, when perceptual distractor identification was less demanding, elevated state anxiety was associated with a heightened response to fearful faces in the amygdala and superior temporal sulcus, whereas individuals scoring high in trait anxiety showed a reduced prefrontal response to these stimuli. The latter finding was interpreted to indicate a weakened recruitment of cortical control mechanisms in anxious individuals when confronted with threat distractors. However, neither high-anxious nor low-anxious volunteers showed an increased amygdala response to threat distractors when the perceptual identification task was more attention demanding (high perceptual load).

Thus, it seems that the findings in subjects showing high, but subclinical, levels of state and trait anxiety may not necessarily be comparable to those of a subject sample meeting the diagnostic criteria for a clinically relevant anxiety disorder such as specific phobia. This difference suggests an increased responsiveness of the amygdala to threat signals in anxiety disorder patients. This increased responsiveness might be mainly associated with differences in the threat relevance of stimuli used in the different studies. Thus, while fearful facial expressions are associated with rather low anxiety ratings, disorder-related stimuli evoke strong fear responses in subjects suffering from an anxiety disorder. Furthermore, our findings are also not associated with trait or state anxiety scores of subjects, since there was no difference between groups. Thus, the use of increased trait or state scores as analogue to clinical disorders might be often of limited relevance. Rather, it may be the disorder-related importance of the stimuli that determines differential brain responses, at least in phobias.

Our results support previous findings of amygdala activation to threat under conditions of attentional distraction in specific phobia [8, 33]. However, no previous study in individuals with anxiety disorders employed a parametric manipulation of attentional load as yet. Also, prior work may differ in that the distraction conditions might not have been very demanding [8], or that they were confounded with other factors [33]. The present results suggest that, at least in specific phobia, the salience of stimuli evokes differential amygdala activation to threat vs neutral stimuli independent of attentional load, even though the amygdala and other areas were found to be modulated by attentional load in general. Thus, high load led to decreased activation of the amygdala and several other brain areas. Conversely, regions implicated in attentional control and dealing with task difficulty showed increased activation under high as compared to low load. This general effect of load or attentional distraction is in line with prior work [15, 17, 23].

We did not detect any evidence for a decreased prefrontal control of threat distractors as suggested by Bishop et al. However, one has to keep in mind that the results in the Bishop et al. study are based on a correlation with trait anxiety scores and a comparable significance of the facial expressions for all subjects (high and low anxious). Here, we compared subjects with anxiety disorder to healthy controls. That is, for spider-phobic subjects the spider pictures were disorder related, while for the control group the (attentional control of these) pictures had no relevance. This prevents a meaningful comparison of differential control mechanisms between groups.

Beyond its role in the rapid induction of defense behaviors [1, 2], the amygdala might also be involved in attentional functions [2, 42–45], for example, through the modulation of activation in visual areas by feedback connections [46]. This influence of the amygdala might allow the enhanced perception of threat [47]. Accordingly, it has been shown that the amygdala drives the activation of areas within the inferior temporo-occipital cortex such as the fusiform gyrus [14] and increased activations to threat even under distraction conditions or perceptual unawareness have been found in visual areas [48, 49]. In line with these findings, our data revealed a significant activation of the fusiform gyrus to spider vs neutral stimuli in spider-phobic subjects occurring in conjunction with the amygdala activation during both attentional conditions.

The amygdala's influence on attentional functions is not specific for anxious subjects or anxiety disorders, but can be found in healthy subjects as well (for example, [43, 44, 50]). Animal research also implicates the amygdala in forming a crucial part of a pervasive vigilance system subserving facilitated processing of biologically relevant information [45, 51, 52]. Thus, the meaning of automatic amygdala activations for phobic symptomatology might be associated with such functions. Individuals suffering from specific phobias show increased vigilance for phobia-relevant stimuli [48, 53]. Under divided attention conditions, the amygdala might be activated even by crude representations of threat stimuli requiring the brain to gather more information by potentiating subsequent sensory information processing.

It should be noted that we do not suggest that these findings are necessarily specific for the processing of phobogenic stimuli. Rather, the processing of phobogenic stimuli represents a highly aversive condition and might be a specific case where personally relevant and salient aversive stimuli are processed even during high perceptual load. Generally, we suggest that activation of the amygdala and visual cortex is due to the interplay between the saliency of stimuli and available cognitive resources. Thus, other threat stimuli might be processed in non-clinical populations as well, given that the saliency and the personal importance of these stimuli are sufficiently high. Future studies should use negative and positive affective control stimuli in order to disentangle the general role of valence and arousal for amygdala responses under high perceptual load.

Furthermore, there was a remarkable reduction of the activation of the amygdala by high load regardless of group and picture category. This is in accordance with previous work (for example, [16]) and provides clear evidence that even in the absence of emotional stimuli the activation in the amygdala is affected by attentional conditions. However, in our study, the differential activation to phobia-related vs neutral pictures was stable across load conditions, indicating a dissociation between a general decrease of the amygdala responsiveness regardless of the specific stimuli and intact relative increased amygdala activation to phobia-related vs neutral stimuli during high load.

Remarkably, there were no effects on task performance in spider-phobic subjects as compared to healthy subjects. However, this finding is in accordance with previous results [8]. Furthermore, impairments in task performance are not consistently observed in subjects with phobias (for example, [7, 8, 54, 55]). Moreover, for the kind of task used in the present study, Bishop et al. showed differential brain activation in anxious subjects that was not accompanied by indications of behavioral impairment. Thus, effects on brain activation can be dissociated from those on behavioral measures, at least when assessed through reaction times and errors. Future studies should investigate whether amygdala responses can predict other behavioral measures. Furthermore, it would be interesting to investigate whether automatic amygdala activations can be modified by successful psychotherapy and if these responses are associated with therapeutic success in the short and long term.

Conclusions

Our results indicate a hyper-responsiveness of the amygdala to disorder-related stimuli in phobic subjects that proved to be independent of attentional load when using a task which induces a high load and which has been shown to prevent amygdala activation to threat in high anxious subjects. This suggests that anxiety disorder patients are characterized by a high level of automaticity of their amygdala responsiveness. Although we did not find an effect of perceptual load on differential amygdala responses, future work might aim to investigate whether a further increase of perceptual load may result in different outcomes as revealed in this study. Thus, also in anxiety patients, the amygdala response to threat might be characterized by a relative instead of an absolute automaticity.

References

  1. LeDoux JE: Fear and the brain: where have we been, and where are we going?. Biol Psychiatry. 1998, 44: 1229-1238. 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00282-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Öhman A, Mineka S: Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev. 2001, 108: 483-522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Dilger S, Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Fitzek C, Reichenbach JR, Hecht H, Krieschel S, Gutberlet I, Miltner WH: Brain activation to phobia-related pictures in spider phobic humans: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Letters. 2003, 348: 29-32. 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00647-5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Liberzon I, Taylor SF, Amdur R, Jung TD, Chamberlain KR, Minoshima S, Koeppe RA, Fig LM: Brain activation in PTSD in response to trauma-related stimuli. Biol Psychiatry. 1999, 45: 817-826. 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00246-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Peters PM, Metzger LJ, Dougherty DD, Cannistraro PA, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ, Pitman RK: Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61: 168-176. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Stein MB, Goldin PR, Sareen J, Zorrilla LT, Brown GG: Increased amygdala activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002, 59: 1027-1034. 10.1001/archpsyc.59.11.1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Straube T, Kolassa I T, Glauer M, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Effect of task conditions on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2004, 56: 921-930. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Neural mechanisms of automatic and direct processing of phobogenic stimuli in specific phobia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 59: 162-170. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Anderson AK, Christoff K, Panitz D, De Rosa E, Gabrieli JD: Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. J Neurosci. 2003, 23: 5627-5633.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Buckner RL, Strauss MM, Hyman SE, Rosen BR: Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron. 1996, 17: 875-887. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ: Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998, 20: 947-957. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80476-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Waiting for spiders: brain activation during anticipatory anxiety in spider phobics. Neuroimage. 2007, 37: 1427-1436. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Straube T, Pohlack S, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Differential amygdala activation to negative and positive emotional pictures during an indirect task. Behav Brain Res. 2008, 191: 285-288. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ: Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2001, 30: 829-841. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00328-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Bishop SJ, Jenkins R, Lawrence AD: Neural processing of fearful faces: effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cereb Cortex. 2007, 17: 1595-1603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Lim SL, Padmala S, Pessoa L: Affective learning modulates spatial competition during low-load attentional conditions. Neuropsychologia. 2008, 46: 1267-1278. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG: Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002, 99: 11458-11463. 10.1073/pnas.172403899.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Straube T, Weiss T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Time course of amygdala activation during aversive conditioning depends on attention. Neuroimage. 2007, 34: 462-469. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lavie N: Perceptual Load as a Necessary Condition for Selective Attention. J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform. 1995, 21: 451-468.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Lavie N: Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005, 9: 75-82. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG: Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007, 7: 336-353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Posner MI, Petersen SE: The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990, 13: 25-42. 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Pessoa L, Padmala S, Morland T: Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage. 2005, 28: 249-255. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.048.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Lavie N, Ro T, Russell C: The role of perceptual load in processing distractor faces. Psych Sci. 2003, 14: 510-515. 10.1111/1467-9280.03453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Mathews A, Mackintosh B: A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cogn Ther Res. 1998, 22: 539-560. 10.1023/A:1018738019346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Pull CB: Recent trends in the study of specific phobias. Curr Opin Psychiatr. 2008, 21: 43-50. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3282f30086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Larson CL, Schaefer HS, Siegle GJ, Jackson CA, Anderle MJ, Davidson RJ: Fear is fast in phobic individuals: amygdala activation in response to fear-relevant stimuli. Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 60: 410-417. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.079.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Wendt J, Lotze M, Weike AI, Hosten N, Hamm AO: Brain activation and defensive response mobilization during sustained exposure to phobia-related and other affective pictures in spider phobia. Psychophysiology. 2008, 45: 205-215. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00620.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. LeDoux JE: Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000, 23: 155-184. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Sabatinelli D, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Lang PJ: Parallel amygdala and inferotemporal activation reflect emotional intensity and fear relevance. Neuroimage. 2005, 24: 1265-1270. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Schienle A, Schäfer A, Walter B, Stark R, Vaitl D: Brain activation of spider phobics towards disorder-relevant, generally disgust- and fear-inducing pictures. Neurosci Lett. 2005, 388: 1-6. 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Straube T, Glauer M, Dilger S, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on brain activation in specific phobia. Neuroimage. 2006, 29: 125-135. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Alpers GW, Gerdes AB, Lagarie B, Tabbert K, Vaitl D, Stark R: Attention and amygdala activity: an fMRI study with spider pictures in spider phobia. J Neural Transm. 2009, 116: 747-757. 10.1007/s00702-008-0106-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1995, Washington, DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association, 4

    Google Scholar 

  35. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T, SKID: Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. 1997, Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  36. Klorman R, Weerts TC, Hastings JE, Melamed BG, Lang PJ: Psychometric description of some specific-fear questionnaires. Behav Ther. 1974, 5: 401-409. 10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80008-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). 1981, Weinheim, Germany: Beltz

    Google Scholar 

  38. Öhman A, Soares JJ: On the automatic nature of phobic fear: conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol. 1993, 102: 121-132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Soares JJ, Öhman A: Preattentive processing, preparedness and phobias: effects of instruction on conditioned electrodermal responses to masked and non-masked fear-relevant stimuli. Behav Res Ther. 1993, 31: 87-95. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90046-W.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lancaster JL, Rainey LH, Summerlin JL, Freitas CS, Fox PT, Evans AC, Toga AW, Mazziotta JC: Automated labeling of the human brain: a preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward-transform method. Hum Brain Mapp. 1997, 5: 238-242. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:4<238::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-4.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Kochunov PV, Nickerson D, Mikiten SA, Fox PT: Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000, 10: 120-131. 10.1002/1097-0193(200007)10:3<120::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: Common and distinct brain activation to threat and safety signals in social phobia. Neuropsychobiology. 2005, 52: 163-168. 10.1159/000087987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR: A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature. 2005, 433: 68-72. 10.1038/nature03086.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Straube T, Dietrich C, Mothes-Lasch M, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH: The volatility of the amygdala response to masked fearful eyes. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010, 31: 1601-1608. 10.1002/hbm.20960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Davis M, Whalen PJ: The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry. 2001, 6: 13-34. 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Amaral DG, Insausti R: Retrograde transport of D-[H-3]-aspartate injected into the monkey amygdaloid complex. Exp Brain Res. 1992, 88: 375-388. 10.1007/BF02259113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Anderson AK, Phelps EA: Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature. 2001, 411: 305-309. 10.1038/35077083.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lipka J, Miltner WH, Straube T: Vigilance for threat interacts with amygdala responses to subliminal threat cues in specific phobia. Biological Psychiatry. 2011, 70: 472-478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ: Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 30: 829-841. 200

  50. Gamer M, Bartosz Z, Büchel C: Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010, 107: 9400-9405. 10.1073/pnas.1000985107.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Holland PC, Gallagher M: Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes. Trends Cog Sci. 1999, 3: 65-73. 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01271-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kapp BS, Whalen PJ, Supple WF, Pascoe JP: Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned arousal and sensory information processing. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. Edited by: Aggleton JP. 1992, New York, USA: Wiley-Liss, 229-254.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Watts FN, Sharrock R: Questionnaire dimensions of spider phobia. Behav Res Ther. 1984, 22: 575-580. 10.1016/0005-7967(84)90061-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Quadflieg S, Mohr A, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH, Straube T: Modulation of the neural network involved in the processing of anger prosody: the role of task-relevance and social phobia. Biol Psychol. 2008, 78: 129-137. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.01.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Schmidt S, Mohr A, Miltner WH, Straube T: Task-dependent neural correlates of the processing of verbal threat-related stimuli in social phobia. Biol Psychol. 2010, 84: 304-312. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (STR 987/2-1; 987/2-3).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Straube.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

TS participated in the design and the data analysis of the study and drafted the manuscript. AS and JL carried out the experiments. AS and MML established the experimental procedures. AS performed the data preprocessing and analysis and wrote parts of the manuscript. WM participated in the development and coordination of the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Straube, T., Lipka, J., Sauer, A. et al. Amygdala activation to threat under attentional load in individuals with anxiety disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 1, 12 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-1-12

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-1-12

Keywords