Skip to main content

Investigating the genetic and environmental bases of biases in threat recognition and avoidance in children with anxiety problems

Abstract

Background

Adults with anxiety show biased categorization and avoidance of threats. Such biases may emerge through complex interplay between genetics and environments, occurring early in life. Research on threat biases in children has focuses on a restricted range of biases, with insufficient focus on genetic and environmental origins. Here, we explore differences between children with and without anxiety problems in under-studied areas of threat bias. We focused both on associations with anxious phenotype and the underlying gene-environmental correlates for two specific processes: the categorisation of threat faces and avoidance learning.

Method

Two-hundred and fifty 10-year old MZ and DZ twin pairs (500 individuals) completed tasks assessing accuracy in the labelling of threatening facial expressions and in the acquisition of avoidant responses to a card associated with a masked threatening face. To assess whether participants met criteria for an anxiety disorder, parents of twins completed a self-guided computerized version of the Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA). Comparison of MZ and DZ twin correlations using model-fitting were used to compute estimates of genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects.

Results

Of the 500 twins assessed, 25 (5%) met diagnostic criteria for a current anxiety disorder. Children with anxiety disorders were more accurate in their ability to recognize disgust faces than those without anxiety disorders, but were commensurate on identifying other threatening face emotions (angry, fearful, sad). Children with anxiety disorders but also more strongly avoided selecting a conditioned stimulus than non-anxious children. While recognition of socially threatening faces was moderately heritable, avoidant responses were heavily influenced by the non-shared environment.

Conclusion

These data add to other findings on threat biases in anxious children. Specifically, we found biases in the labelling of some negative-valence faces and in the acquisition of avoidant responses. While non-shared environmental effects explained all of the variance on threat avoidance, some of this may be due to measurement error.

Background

Anxiety disorders are common [1], disabling and costly for society [2]. As most adult anxiety disorders have their roots in childhood [1], there is an urgent need to understand the mechanisms by which child anxiety develops. Interventions administered in childhood may also have longer-lasting benefits on anxiety outcomes [3]. Nevertheless, childhood anxiety remains under-studied [4]. Threat biases are a key component of adult anxiety. Such threat biases may arise from inherited perturbations in brain circuitry functioning [57], yet are also relatively plastic, developing in response to environmental experiences [8]. Compared to adults, the few available studies on threat biases in anxious children have focused on a more restricted range of processing biases. Less is also known about the origins of processing biases in children as compared to adults. Given vast biological, cognitive and social differences between children and adults [9, 10], one cannot simply extrapolate data on adults to children with anxiety. In this manuscript we: (i) follow-up cross-sectional links between less frequently-studied threat biases in our own sample of children with and without anxiety disorders; and (ii) investigate the genetic and environmental origins of some of these anxiety-based threat biases.

Threat bias research in adults and children

Extensive data from anxious adults suggest biases at various stages of information-processing. Studies have identified biases in the initial orienting towards threat cues, i.e. attention capture e.g. [11] and in the inability to disengage attention away from threat cues i.e. attention control e.g. [12, 13], using both word and picture (including face) stimuli. Studies have also focused on the tendency to label neutral materials as threatening, for example by drawing threatening interpretations of ambiguous stories or words, and showing greater recognition of threatening facial expressions [14, 15]. An emergent body of work has also considered biases in associative learning, particularly in the acquisition, generalisation, and extinction of fear from aversive unconditioned stimuli to neutral cues that signal threat, but also cues that signal safety [16]. These biases in threat-learning shape other biases, as they relate to subsequent behavioural responses [17, 18], in which avoidant responses are acquired to attenuate aversive fear-states associated with the threat cue [19]. In summary, adult data document anxiety-based threat biases at various stages of information-processing, from attention to behaviour.

In contrast to the wealth of data on biased threat processing in adults with anxiety, pediatric studies have focused on a restricted range of processing biases. These are presented in Tables 123. To our knowledge, over 30 studies (Table 1) have measured biases in attention-capture and control to threatening words and pictures (including faces) in children and adolescents. These studies have capitalized on a range of experimental tasks tapping distinct attention processes. The largest number of studies has used the visual probe task, followed by the emotional Stroop task, then the visual search and affective Go/No-Go tasks. Results from the visual probe task generally support robust effects of anxiety on vigilance for threatening words/pictures, with only four out of 18 published studies finding no effects of anxiety (n = 1 study) or an opposite pattern of attentional-avoidance (n = 3 studies). While the reasons for these discrepancies remain unclear, preliminary data from adults may be informative. Specifically, these data suggest that conditions of acute stress can lead some individuals to shift their attention away from threats, perhaps to minimize prolonged stress exposure, resulting in a pattern of attentional-avoidance on tasks such as the visual-probe [20]. Given these data, it is noteworthy that among two of the studies finding contradictory results, one sampled children with PTSD following maltreatment [21] and the other was conducted during an anxiety-provoking brain scan [22]. These hypotheses, while intriguing, require further investigation. Studies using the visual search design find signs of hypervigilance to threat amongst children with anxiety problems, and most studies using the emotional Stroop have found interference effects from salient threat cues. Similarly, while the Go/No-Go task is primarily used as a measure of response inhibition, the inclusion of emotional stimuli as ‘No Go’ cues has also yielded anxiety-based interference effects from threats. In sum, studies of attention-capture and control are generally supportive of disruptions in attention resources in the presence of threat, manifesting as vigilance towards threat or interference that is associated with the presence of threat.

Table 1 Studies of biased attention to threat in anxious youth with studies presenting conflicting results to adult data italicised
Table 2 Studies of biased selection of threat interpretations in anxious youth with studies presenting conflicting results to adult data italicised
Table 3 Studies of biased fear and avoidance learning in anxious children with studies presenting conflicting results to adult data italicised

Nearly as many studies (n = 30 in Table 2) have explored biases in the labelling of material as threatening. These studies draw on many different methodologies, including presenting participants with ambiguous scenarios, words and pictures, to which participants’ tendency to select threat or benign interpretations is assessed. Other tasks require participants to label face-emotions displays. Studies assessing the evaluation of ambiguous scenarios are clear in suggesting that children with anxiety draw more threatening interpretations of hypothetical situations than their peers with fewer anxiety problems, mirroring adult data. Similarly, there is generally good support for these patterns in the endorsement of threat-meanings of ambiguous words and pictures, such as in homophones or homographs. Data regarding the labelling of threatening facial expressions can be divided into those assessing the rating of fear towards face emotions and those measuring the misclassification of different expressions. The data suggest that children with anxiety do not rate negative faces as more fear-provoking than children without anxiety. Nevertheless, between-group differences arise in the categorisation of various negative but also positive facial expressions. These data, while intriguing are few. Moreover, these results contrast with data in adults with anxiety, who are superior at identifying angry and fearful faces than adults without anxiety [14, 85]. However, they require further clarification in other samples.

Finally, a handful of studies (n = 6, Table 3) have begun to explore biases in the acquisition and extinction of fear and avoidance of conditioned threat stimuli (CS+), paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (UCS), and of conditioned safety stimuli (CS-) not paired with the UCS. Together these studies tentatively suggest that there are anxiety-based differences in the learning and retention of fear to neutral stimuli paired with UCSs (i.e. the CS+). Moreover, these persistent fears can generalize to neutral stimuli that are unpaired with the UCS (i.e. the CS-). Only one study has investigated anxiety links with the acquisition of avoidant responses to neutral cues associated with the UCS [84]. This study demonstrated that children with anxiety were more likely to avoid selecting the CS+, a red (or yellow) coloured card that was paired with a masked threatening face, when asked to choose one of two cards to win points. These differences associated with high levels of anxiety were of moderately large effect size (Table 3) but await further verification in other samples.

At first, the findings on avoidant-learning may seem to contradict other data showing that children with anxiety are more vigilant for threat cues in visual probe tasks, compared to their peers with fewer anxiety problems (Table 1). However, the tasks from which these two sets of findings arise may tap different stages in processing. Thus, one could consider the visual probe task to measure biases in ‘attention capture’, the initial, automatic attention-orienting towards threat cues. In contrast, the avoidance paradigm used here may tap response-selection or even behavioral enactment, that is, the degree to which children act to deselect a cue that has an acquired threat value. Given these differences between tasks, the data may instead point to a pattern of initial vigilance (demonstrated by visual probe tasks that present threat stimuli briefly) followed by avoidance (demonstrated by the avoidance-learning task where children either select or deselect a cue signalling threat) among children with anxiety. Such a pattern has also been described in adult data (see [86] for a review), in which individuals with anxiety problems tend to be hypervigilant to briefly presented threat cues, and avoidant of threat cues presented under longer durations.

In summary, there is a large corpus of data investigating attentional mechanisms in children with anxiety. While there are also many studies focusing on interpretations of ambiguous scenarios and words, far fewer studies have measured anxiety-based differences in the categorisation of non-verbal threatening stimuli, such as negative face-emotions. There is also a dearth of research investigating threat-learning difficulties in general and the acquisition of avoidance learning in particular. Given these gaps, the current study aimed to verify the direction of less well-studied anxiety-linked processing biases in youth by comparing children with and without anxiety problems in the categorisation of threatening faces, including angry, fearful, sad, and disgust facial expressions; and in the acquisition of avoidance of cues signaling threat. Mixed results precluded specific hypotheses on the direction and nature of recognition biases, but enhanced tendencies to engage avoidant response styles were predicted from one other prior study [84].

Developmental origins of threat biases

Very few studies have clarified the origins of putative information-processing biases in children with anxiety. In adults, threat biases may arise from genetic variation, as revealed through preliminary candidate-gene studies [5] and family studies [54] of anxiety-based biases. However threat biases can clearly be shaped by exposure to environmental experiences, as illustrated by individuals who have recently experienced a traumatic event. Data from cognitive bias modification studies also show that external training paradigms can be used to manipulate threat biases in attention and interpretation [87]. Given that there may be changes in the expression of particular anxiety-genes across development [10], and in the salience of particular environmental factors between children and adults [88], simple extrapolation of adult data to understanding anxiety in children is not valid.

Many models of child anxiety suggest that threat biases arise from both family and child characteristics [54, 8991]. In spite of this, empirical work has typically focused on the so-called ‘nurture’ aspect, noting that family interaction patterns in general and parenting styles in particular predict response strategies among anxious offspring [54, 9294]. Apart from work on associations between temperamental traits and information-processing, very few studies have examined the role of ‘nature’ on threat biases in children. In one study, we found moderate genetic effects on threat interpretation of ambiguous words and scenarios [59]. In another study of the same sample, moderate genetic contributions were also found for labelling of various threatening facial expressions, including fearful, sadness, and disgust [95]. In the current manuscript, we extended exploration of genetic and environmental effects to another information-processing factor relevant to anxiety, acquisition of avoidance to a threat cue, also in the sample. Consistent with existing theories of childhood anxiety and our previous findings, we predicted joint roles of environmental and genetic factors.

Present article

The present study had two goals. The focus of the first set of analysis reported here was to identify whether there were anxiety-based group differences on understudied threat biases: the categorisation of threat faces and in avoidance learning. While both sets of analyses were conducted in a twin sample, we focused our comparison on children with anxiety disorders (specifically, those meeting symptom-threshold and reporting clinical distress and impairment) and those who had never met criteria for an anxiety disorder and who reported low levels of anxiety symptoms. We compared these ‘extreme’ groups to maximize anxiety group differences, and to make our findings maximally relevant to clinical samples. Our second set of analyses estimated genetic and environmental influences on threat biases that appeared to play a role in child anxiety. These analyses were conducted in a larger twin sample because analyses that partition individual variability into several sources of influence rely on larger samples that include both MZ and DZ twins, with a full range of scores on these measures. The smaller sub-sample in the first set of analysis would not have yielded enough power to detect genetic and environmental influences with confidence.

Method

Participants

Subjects were 250 pairs of 10 year-old twins from Wave 2 of the Emotions, Cognitions, Heredity and Outcomes (ECHO) study. Participants of this study were initially selected using an extremes design from a large longitudinal sample of twins born in England and Wales [96] to target children with high levels of emotional symptoms. The Wave 1 sample comprised: 247 8 year-old twin pairs selected for high scores on parent-reported anxiety at age 7 years; and 53 randomly selected ‘control twin pairs’ [97], in which neither of the children within the pair scored in the top 5% of the anxiety score distribution in TEDS at age 7. These control pairs were included to ensure coverage of the full range of scores on test measures. A total of 250 twin pairs returned for Wave 2 [98], forming the subject pool for the current analyses. The reduction in numbers between Waves 1 and 2 was because of attrition and because 11 families were considered unusable at Wave 1, due to autistic spectrum disorders, severe receptive language impairments, and persistent attention problems in at least one of the twins (2 of these 11 families were control pairs). Of these twin pairs, 203 pairs were those selected for high scores on parentreported anxiety at age 7, and the remaining 47 pairs were those initially selected as ‘control’ pairs. At the time of the Wave 2 visit, twins were aged between 9 years 7 months and 10 years 10 months (mean: 10 years 1 month). A total of 56.4% of the sample was female, with 83 monozygotic (MZ) and 167 dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs. Informed consent was obtained from parents of twins. Ethical approval for this study was given by the Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley NHS Trust.

To assess current psychiatric status, parents of twins completed a self-guided computerized version of a structured interview concerning their child’s behaviour at age 10 years: Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA, [99]). This interview was conducted at the Wave 2 visit, and contained items assessing not only the presence but also severity and impairment of psychiatric symptoms. Computer algorithms generated preliminary DSM-IV diagnoses. In addition to these items, free text boxes allowed parents to include supplementary details. Together with computer-generated diagnoses, these were used by one of the authors of this study (RG), who was also the first author of the DAWBA interview, and a highly experienced clinician, to make the final diagnosis, blind to all other information. In another study, inter-rater reliability for the presence of any DSM-IV diagnosis was =0.86 across 500 subjects [100], with = 0.67 for agreement between DAWBA diagnoses and diagnoses from casenotes [99]. No other diagnostic instruments were employed to validate diagnoses. However, subjects meeting criteria for an anxiety diagnosis had significantly higher scores on the Screen for Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) [101] than subjects who did not meet criteria (mean scores = 30.00 versus 24.89, Δ 2 (1) = 9.46, p < 0.01 when comparing sub-modelsthat tested mean differences).

Of the 500 twins (250 twin pairs) seen at Wave 2, 25 (5%) met criteria for a current anxiety disorder, while 437 received no diagnoses for any psychiatric disorder. Of the 25 anxious subjects, 2 had generalized anxiety, 7 had separation anxiety, 8 had specific phobia and 10 had an anxiety disorder not otherwise specified; one met criteria for a comorbid depressive disorder and 5 other met criteria for externalizing disorders. Because of the extremes design used at Wave 1 to select symptomatic subjects to the study, some of the 437 individuals who did not receive anxiety diagnoses could still manifest high levels of anxiety symptoms even though they did not meet clinical criteria. Indeed, as reported above, these individuals had an overall mean score of 24.89 on the SCARED, which approaches the clinical cut-off on this measure (total score > 25). As such, using these individuals does not comprise an appropriate comparison group for testing anxiety-linked differences on test variables. Instead, we removed those individuals who were initially selected for having high scores on the age 7 parent-reported anxiety measure, leaving only subjects who were initially designated ‘control pairs’ at Wave 1 as comparison subjects in our group analysis of threat measures. Of the original 106 control twins (53 twin pairs), only 94 (47 twin pairs) were retained at Wave 2. This was because of attrition (4 families) and because 2 families were not followed up due to presence of autistic spectrum disorders, severe receptive language impairments, and persistent attention problems in at least one of the twins. One individual from these 94 children met criteria for an anxiety disorder at Wave 2 and was grouped in the anxious group; their co-twin was also removed as a comparative subject in these analyses. The mean score on the SCARED for the remaining 92 ‘control twins’ was 21.86 (SD = 9.95). As such, these comparison individuals reflected those who had: (i) not fallen in the top 5% of age 7 anxiety scores, (ii) did not meet diagnostic criteria for an anxiety disorder at age 10 years, and (iii) had fewer anxiety symptoms, as measured by the SCARED than those in the diagnostic group at age 10.

Tasks

Two tasks assessing biases in the interpretation of face emotions and in avoidance learning were administered.

The recognition of facial affect task measured ability to correctly label five face emotions: happiness, anger, fear, sad and disgust. However, because of our interest in threat faces, trials in which happy faces were presented were excluded from these data analysis. On each trial, subjects were instructed to label each facial expression by selecting one of five labels that corresponded to the different emotions (happy, anger, sad, fear, disgust) with their computer mouse. Once the subject had clicked on a label, the next trial was presented. The task was therefore self-pacing with variable inter-stimulus intervals across trials and across participants. Facial expression morphs were displayed as animations changing from the neutral expression (0%) to one of four levels of intensity (25%, 50%, 75% or 100%). Accurate responses were scored 1, and inaccurate responses were scored 0. The task comprised 160 trials. Thus, there were 32 trials of each face-emotion, with 8 trials for each of the four intensity levels. These 8 trials were further divided into 4 trial-types (Figure 1). Specifically, head orientation (facing i.e. frontal or sideways i.e. profile to the camera) and eye-gaze direction (towards or away from the camera) of faces was manipulated to create these 4 trial-types: head facing-eyes towards; head facing-eyes away; head sideways-eyes towards; and head sideways-eyes away. The manipulations were used to increase task difficulty, ensuring greater individual variability on accuracy scores for subsequent twin analysis and to avoid ceiling effects (i.e. with all participants correctly identifying all expressions). As expected, across all participants in the whole sample trials in which the head faced the subject yielded greater accuracy relative to when the head was presented from a side angle (F(1,247) = 8.28, p < .01). Similarly, greater accuracy characterized trials in which the eyes appeared toward rather than away from the subject across all participants in the whole sample (F(1,247) = 22.93 p < .001). Finally, half of the images were drawn from one male actor, and the other half, from one female actor. In summary, trials varied across a number of variables: gender of the face, facial expression, intensity, head orientation and eye gaze direction with equal numbers of each combination, randomly ordered.

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic of the face emotion recognition task with the different expression intensities and eye gaze and head orientation conditions; only angry faces are shown for brevity.

Prior to commencing the task, subjects were read standardized instructions. To ensure all subjects were familiar with the 5 emotions, they were asked to provide a definition of each emotion. Subjects were also given 5 practice trials consisting of the faces of two individuals (one male and one female) not used elsewhere in the experiment, demonstrating the five expressions animated from neutral to full-blown intensity. Accuracy scores were summed separately for angry, fear, sad and disgust facial expressions comprising the dependent variables for these analyses. As each face emotion was presented across 32 trials, a score of 32 reflects 100% accuracy. Similar tasks employing computer-generated animation of facial expressions have differentiated anxious from non-anxious adults [14].

Variations of the present task have been used to study face emotion recognition in a variety of populations, ranging from normal volunteers (of different ages) to individuals with psychopathology, genetic disorders or medical conditions. These results suggest that the paradigm is sensitive to subtle age-related trajectories and disorder-linked impairments of emotion perception, compared to tasks employing static images (see [102] for details). The facial expressions of the male and female actor used in the present study were also drawn from these same studies [103, 104]. In brief, these individuals were selected from 26 individuals as having the most recognizable expressions of the four basic emotions see [103]; in this same study, which also presented computer generated morphs of the ‘intermediate’ expressions (i.e. displaying different intensities), healthy adults’ accuracy scores ranged from approximately 70-100%, suggesting that facial expressions were valid. Facial expressions were presented in colour. The actors were young adults of Caucasian ethnicity.

The avoidance learning task was designed to assess whether participants associated a neutral stimulus (a colored card) with a threatening stimulus (an angry face) and subsequently avoided this stimulus across trials [105]. On each trial, subjects were presented with two packs of cards (red and yellow). Standardized instructions indicated that the goal was to choose between the coloured packs to win points. Subjects were led to believe that one colour was associated with more points and advised to sample from both packs to decide which was more advantageous. Once they were reasonably sure, subjects were encouraged to continue with that pack for the remainder of the task. In reality subjects’ choice of colored card was systematically associated with the subsequent presentation of an angry or neutral facial expression, rather than being associated with high or low reward points. In fact, reward points were either 2358, 2361, 2459, or 2463, chosen to increase the difficulty in tracking points. The face expression followed card-color choice and was presented for 30 ms, which was then followed by a gray face-shaped oval mask (200 ms). Prior studies have found that these presentation conditions can result in a reduced awareness of the facial expression, that is, subjects only report seeing the mask but not the face [106].

This task consisted of five blocks of 32 trials. The number of responses to the color card associated with the angry face in each block formed the dependent test variable indexing avoidance. As each block contained 32 trials, a score of 32 reflects 100% avoidant responses, whereas 50% reflects chance-responding. Color-face pairings were counterbalanced across subjects: for some subjects, the yellow card presented on the left side was always paired with the angry face while the red card, presented on the right side was paired with the neutral face. Other subjects received reversed pairings such that the red card on the left was paired with the angry face and the yellow card on the right with the neutral face. Facial expressions were taken from a standard set of pictures of facial affect, presented in black and white [107].

Data analysis

The structural equation modeling package Mx [108] was used to conduct two sets of analyses on our sample of twins. First, we analyzed differences between children with and without anxiety problems on threat measures, by comparing children who met diagnostic criteria for an anxiety disorder and those who did not meet criteria and who did not experience high levels of anxiety symptoms. Second we explored genetic and environmental influences on threat measures that showed significant differences in the first set of analysis. While the first set of analysis used a sub-sample of ECHO, the second set focussed on all twins in Wave 2. In the first set of analyses, we compared the 25 individuals meeting criteria for current anxiety diagnoses with the 92 individuals selected as control twins at age 7 years. We used a model-fitting approach to test for differences between children with and without anxiety problems because these analyses can control for the non-independence of data from related individuals. Specifically, we compared nested models that either assumed that means for a variable were the same or different for children with and without anxiety. Thus, anxiety-linked differences on threat measures were ascertained by comparing a model that estimated separate means for each group, with a sub-model equating means across the sample. The fit of a model, i.e. the extent to which estimated parameters reflect observed statistics, is indexed by the Chi-square (2) value relative to the degrees of freedom. This 2 value is derived from the difference in the loglikelihood (−2LL) statistic generated from comparison of tested models with saturated models (which estimate the means, variances and covariances of all data). Lower, non-significant 2 values indicate good model-fit. Another consideration of model-fitting is parsimony, which means that of two models of equal fit the one with fewer parameters is preferred. The Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC), which is calculated as 2-2df is an index of both fit and parsimony, with more negative values indicating a well-fitting and parsimonious model.

Differences between children with and without anxiety problems were therefore reflected by assessing whether there were significant differences in model-fit between models that estimated separate or the same mean across groups for a particular measure indexed through changes in 2 relative to changes in degrees of freedom (df). Model-fitting techniques to assess between group differences can control for shared variance between family members by estimating the covariance between twin 1 and twin 2 variables. Complementary mixed design ANOVA analyses were also performed to ensure similar results as those found from model-fitting approaches. For the face emotion recognition responses, ‘emotion’ was included as a within-subjects factor and ‘anxiety group’ as a between-subjects factor. For the avoidance learning data, we included ‘block’ as the within-subject factor and again anxiety-group as the between-subjects factor. Groups did not differ on key demographic variables of age, gender and ethnicity ratio, and socioeconomic status (SES) (Table 4). SES was assessed using the TEDS composite score based on qualifications and current employment for both parents, and mother's age at the birth of her first child.

Table 4 Demographic, diagnostic and threat measure variables across children with anxious problems (n = 25) and without (n = 92) subjects

The second set of analyses used data from the entire Wave 2 sample (n = 500) to explore genetic and environmental influences on threat measures that showed significant differences in our first set of analysis. As we have already reported on genetic and environmental influences on classification of face-emotions in this sample, here, we just focused on avoidance acquisition. As this sample over-selected for children with high anxiety scores, it is likely that means are increased, while variances and covariance of correlated variables are decreased [109]. Thus in estimating genetic and environmental influences on threat measures, a weighting system was used. A weight controlling for ascertainment bias was first constructed using the ratio of the selection probability of families of children with high symptom scores to that of ‘control’ families of children with lower symptom scores among ECHO participants. A second weight controlling for attrition bias was then made using significant predictors of the probability of families remaining at Wave 2. Significant predictors included individuals with mothers reporting higher levels of emotional symptoms and who experienced greater negative life events being less likely to participate. These weights were multiplied and included in analyses to compensate for unequal response rates among individuals from different population strata. During model-fitting procedures, which use maximum likelihood methods to estimate parameters, less weight is applied to individuals from categories overrepresented and more weight to individuals from categories underrepresented in the ECHO sample, relative to the larger more representative, TEDS sample.

Genetic, shared environmental and non-shared environmental effects are estimated through comparisons of within-pair similarity among MZ twins, who share 100% of their genetic makeup and DZ twins who share on average 50% of segregating genes. Within-pair similarity is typically indexed by twin correlations. Higher MZ compared to DZ similarity is attributed to the increased genetic resemblance among MZ twins, and used to estimate heritability (a2). Within-pair similarity not due to genetic factors is assigned as shared environmental variance (c2), which contributes towards resemblance among individuals reared in the same family. Finally, non-shared environmental influences (e2) are individual-specific experiences differing among individuals from the same family, and are estimated from within-pair differences between MZ twins (1 - MZ twin correlations). This term also includes measurement error. As before 2 and AIC values were used to assess fit and parsimony of these models.

Results

Anxiety-based differences in recognition of facial affect

Table 4 presents for each group, the total number of correct responses to each face-emotion across all trials, with a total number of 32 trials of each face-emotion. Equivocal evidence arose for an anxiety-related deficit in face-emotion identification. Subjects with and without anxiety problems were comparable in recognizing angry, fear and sad faces (Table 4 for comparison of fit statistics across models). However children with an anxiety diagnosis were better than children without an anxiety 2 diagnosis at identifying disgust faces (comparison of fit statistics across models: Δ (1) = 6.52, p < 0.05). Complementary repeated measures ANOVA did find a main effect of emotion (F(3,348) = 11.08, p < .001) but a non-significant emotion-by-anxiety-group interaction (F(3,348) = 2.17, p = .1). We next examined group differences associated with anxiety problems for each emotion separately. As with the model-fitting data, independent sample t-tests showed that children with anxiety problems made fewer errors when labelling disgust but were similar in their accuracy in identifying other facial expressions (t(116) = 2.61, p < .05 for group comparison to disgust faces). Effect sizes of these group differences, calculated using the standardised difference between the means (Cohen’s d), were moderate (Table 4).

Anxiety-based differences in avoidance learning

Table 4 reports, for each group, the total number of responses corresponding to avoidance in each block, with a total number of 32 trials presented in each block. A clear association between anxiety and avoidance emerged: subjects with anxiety disorders were less likely than subjects without anxiety disorders to choose 2 a cue paired with the angry face (comparison of fit statistics across models: Δ (1) = 6.89, p < 0.01). Moreover, differences in these ‘avoidant’ responses emerged across blocks (Table 4). Again, we repeated these analyses using ANOVAs. A main effect of block emerged (F(4,464) = 5.06, p < .01). However the main effect of anxiety only approached a non-significant trend (F(1,116) = 3.18, p = .08). Examining avoidant choices in each block separately yielded main effects of anxiety-group in Block 4 only (t(116) = 2.07, p < .05). These discrepancies in results between model-fitting approaches and mixed design ANOVAs may reflect the use of sampling weights in model-fitting approaches. Effect sizes of these group differences, calculated using the standardised difference between the means (Cohen’s d), were moderate (Table 4).

Of note, to assess when children with and without anxiety problems began to consistently select specific cues, we conducted in each group a series of one sample t tests comparing whether total responses for the card associated with the angry face, per block deviated significantly from 16 trials (i.e. chance responding). For children with anxiety problems, responses from Block 1 and 5 were all significantly below 16 (indicating selection of the card associated with the neutral face); for children without anxiety problems, responses from all blocks were significantly greater than 16 (indicating selection of the card associated with the angry face). This suggests that differential responses are selected between children with and without anxiety problems in the first 32 trials.

Genetic and environmental influences on avoidance learning

Next, we explored genetic and environmental influences on avoidance acquisition. MZ and DZ twin correlations were -.08 and -.03 respectively for avoidance. As both of these were close to zero, they suggested sole influence of non-shared environmental variance/measurement error on avoidance. Model-fitting confirmed these interpretations where a model with non-shared environmental variance at 100% (92-100%) and no genetic (0-7%) or shared environmental effects (0-6%) provided good fit: -2LL = 1021.20, df = 456, 2 (12) = 4.46, AIC = −19.44. A previous study found support for genetic effects with non-shared environmental contributions on disgust recognition in this sample [97]. As only one pair of DZ twins were concordant on having an anxiety disorder, inadequate power precluded estimating genetic and environmental influences on diagnostic data.

Discussion

Biased information-processing for threat material is a key component of anxiety, with many data suggesting biases in attention towards threat and in the interpretation of ambiguous (verbal) material (Tables 1,2,3). Prior studies have demonstrated anxiety-based disruptions on attentional mechanisms, and in the interpretation of verbal ambiguous material, such as words and scenarios. However, there are gaps in the literature with fewer studies measuring differences in the evaluation of non-verbal threatening stimuli, such as negative face-emotions, and in the acquisition of fear and avoidance to potential threat cues.

Here, we aimed to follow up some gaps identified in our knowledge of anxiety-based threat biases in children, by exploring differences in the classification of threatening facial expressions and in the acquisition of avoidant responses to a masked threatening face.

Specifically, within our sample of 10-year old twins, we compared those who had been selected for high anxiety symptoms at age 7 and who also met criteria for an anxiety disorder at age 10, with those who did not meet diagnostic criteria and reported lower anxiety scores at age 7 or 10 years. Clear support for one of our hypotheses was found: subjects with an anxiety problem were more likely to avoid a cue paired with a masked angry facial expression, compared to children without anxiety problems. This replicates a prior study using this task in another sample of children [84]. Interestingly in both studies, this tendency appeared to emerge over the course of the task across blocks between children with and without anxiety problems. Results were less clear for another hypothesis: while the ability to recognize angry, fear, and sad facial expressions was not associated with anxiety, children with an anxiety problem were significantly better than those without problems at identifying disgust faces. Contrary to our predictions on the origins of threat biases, no support was found for genetic effects, and all of the variance was explained by non-shared environmental effects, which could include measurement error. This contrasted with the ability to identify disgust faces, which in a previous study of the same sample showed moderate genetic and large environmental contributions [96], consistent with joint roles for nature and nurture.

These data are subject to various limitations. First, it is possible that the avoidance task is less reliable than the facial recognition task, and that this reduced the twin correlations. This may explain the lack of heritability on threat avoidance, while artificially inflating non-shared environmental contributions which includes measurement error. More particularly, different methodologies for assessing threat biases are likely to have different levels of reliability; an experimental task is likely to be less reliable than a questionnaire measure, resulting in greater non-shared environmental and subsequently smaller genetic influences. Investigating the genetic and environmental origins of such threat biases using experimental tasks rather than questionnaires would therefore benefit from attempts to quantify and improve on the psychometrics of such tasks.

A second set of caveats concerns methods used to ascertain diagnostic status of our ‘anxious’ subjects. While we relied on a version of a well-validated structured interview [99] -the DAWBA - its’ application of the functional impairment criterion described in DSM, as well as meeting symptom-threshold may have under-identified those with anxiety problems reported in the present sample. Indeed other studies have often reported discrepancies in prevalence rates of anxiety disorders depending on whether clinically-significant distress and impairment are incorporated in the diagnostic procedures [110, 111], with far lower rates reported when ‘clinical impact’ is considered. In the present study, using both clinical impact and symptoms to determine diagnosis, 5% of the sample met criteria for an anxiety disorder. While this figure is not particularly low when considered against other reported prevalence rates of this age range, which can vary between 2.6% to 41.2% [4], it is lower than expected given that most children in the sample were selected for high anxiety. This allows the possibility that many more children in our sample had high anxiety, but only some also experienced distress and impairment. Indeed as a group, non-diagnosed children in the ECHO sample reported a mean anxiety score on the SCARED that approached clinical cut-off for that measure. As we were interested in assessing anxiety-based differences in threat biases, it was even more important for us to compare children who met clinical diagnosis with children who not only did not meet clinical diagnosis, but who also reported fewer anxiety problems at age 7 and at age 10. Thus, we selected our original control twin pairs as the comparison subjects for this analysis (rather than simply those who did not meet anxiety disorder at age 10 years). A second issue relates to the use of parent-reported data to generate computerized algorithms to detect children meeting criteria for an anxiety disorder. This approach raises issues on the accuracy of parents as informants, in the absence of clinical interview. Future research should employ multi-method, multi-informant measures to increase the validity of reported associations between anxiety and information-processing biases.

A final issue concerns heterogeneity in diagnostic subtypes and co-morbid conditions within the group meeting criteria for at least one current anxiety disorder. Although collapsing across subtypes is sub-optimal, adequate numbers precluded examination of more specific links with biases in information-processing. Nevertheless studies of anxiety subtypes in children typically yield strong cross-sectional and longitudinal comorbidity; similar mental health histories; and large overlap in genetic liability [112, 113], lending justification to analyzing anxiety disorders as a single group in the first instance.

Despite these limitations, our data offer some interesting extensions into cognitive phenotypes of child anxiety. One key finding relates to the association between anxiety and avoidance. Prior studies examining avoidance in anxiety have used questionnaires or observational measures that do not correspond closely to definitions of established theories [105]. According to such theories, through associative learning, a neutral conditioned stimulus (e.g. color card, CS+) acquires fear-eliciting properties of an aversive unconditioned stimulus (e.g. angry face, UCS). Avoidance is then employed and reinforced through its fear-reducing capacity. The paradigm in the current study was designed to model the acquisition of avoidance learning. Both the current and a prior study using this paradigm [84] show that children with anxiety problems, defined by questionnaire and diagnostic measures, are more likely than children with fewer anxiety problems to ‘avoid’ a cue associated with a threatening face in favor of a cue associated with a neutral face. These data fit in well with suggestions that anxiety in childhood can be characterised by a pattern of initial vigilance (as suggested by visual probe studies showing attention-orienting towards threats) followed by subsequent avoidance in response-selection. Interestingly, this vigilance-avoidance pattern has also been described in adults with anxiety [86]. These findings are not too surprising when placed in the context of clinical features of anxiety disorders in children and adults, which often involve marked fear and avoidance of the feared object in tandem. Indeed avoidant strategies are thought to maintain the marked fear. Finally, using model-fitting estimates, the current study does not support the role of inherited factors in shaping these avoidant behaviours but instead points to the importance of non-shared environmental variance. Future studies should try to assess the contribution of specific, measured environmental influences that account for this source of variance.

A second interesting finding was the association between anxiety and biased recognition of disgust faces, a bias that we previously reported to be influenced by genetic factors [95]. This bias in the recognition of disgust among children with anxiety problems occurred in the context of similar abilities to identify angry, fear and sad expressions relative to children without anxiety. There is some support from pediatric samples corroborating the relationship between trait anxiety and disgust sensitivity [114]. Preliminary data also finds greater sensitivity to disgust in adults with high levels of social anxiety, relative to adults with lower levels. This sensitivity is manifest through behavioral ratings and reaction times to disgust faces as well as in patterns of brain activation [115]. Why would anxious individuals be more sensitive to disgust stimuli? Given that biases associated with threat are a characteristic of children with anxiety, disgust could reflect a social threat (e.g. rejection). Disgust could also signal a physical threat (e.g. contamination). Further work is needed to clarify the role of disgust in children’s anxiety problems. It would also be interesting to explore whether disgust faces similarly affect other stages of information-processing, such as by capturing or interfering with attentional resources.

Increased avoidance of feared stimuli and sensitivity to disgust stimuli may contribute to pathological anxiety in the long-term by maintaining anxious states. Thus therapeutic interventions that aim to extinguish acquired fear and avoidant associations, or modify biases in the processing of threats may be particularly helpful in combating anxiety. Exposure based interventions, which capitalize on fear reduction through extinction learning have been used effectively to treat adult anxiety and to some extent in children with anxiety problems. As fear to the CS + declines via extinction, avoidance will no longer be needed to attenuate fear. Alternatively, children could be taught counter strategies to terminate conditioned fear, such as the use of approach-based strategies. For disgust sensitivity, new bias modification programs could be developed and implemented to train children to re-evaluate initial impressions based on other evidence, such as the presence of positive emotional expressions. While cognitive bias modification training tasks appear effective in manipulating attention away from threats, or leading to the re-appraisal of ambiguous scenarios, these training tasks have not yet been extended to manipulate the labelling of ambiguous non-verbal cues, such as face-emotions. If children with high anxiety do show greater recognition of disgust faces, this could be a new target for such computerized training paradigms - with the aim to reduce negative perceptions and thus anxious mood-states.

Conclusion

In the present study, we systematically examined anxiety-based threat biases in children. Through a brief review of studies, we identified gaps in the range of processing biases explored. To extend this literature, we assessed threat biases in the tendency to label ambiguous face-emotions as threatening, and in the acquisition of avoidance responses to threat cues. Our data showed that children with anxiety problems were more likely to correctly identify disgust faces and to avoid a conditioned stimulus paired with a masked angry face. We also investigated the genetic and environmental origins of these threat biases. While disgust sensitivity, as reported by a previous study was shaped both by inherited characteristics and by individual-specific aspects of the environment, data tentatively suggested that the tendency to acquire avoidance to a threat was heavily influenced by non-shared environmental experiences. These data add to existing findings showing linkages between anxiety in children and the tendency to favour threatening information during information-processing. More generally, they also support the role of nurture on generating these threat biases.

References

  1. Kessler RC, et al: Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of mental disorders in the World Health Organization's World Mental Health Survey Initiative. World Psychiatry. 2007, 6 (3): 168-176.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Greenberg PE, et al: The economic burden of anxiety disorders in the 1990s. JClin Psychiatry. 1999, 60 (7): 427-435. 10.4088/JCP.v60n0702.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Leonardo ED, Hen R: Anxiety as a developmental disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008, 33 (1): 134-140. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301569.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Cartwright-Hatton S, McNicol K, Dobbleday E: Anxiety in a neglected population. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006, 26: 817-833. 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Fox E, Ridgewell A, Ashwin C: Looking on the bright side: biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proc Biol Sci. 2009, 276 (1663): 1747-1751. 10.1098/rspb.2008.1788.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fox E, et al: The serotonin transporter gene alters sensitivity to attention biasmodification (ABM): Evidence for a plasticity gene. Biological Psychiatry. inpress

  7. Hariri AR, et al: Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of thehuman amygdala. Science. 2002, 297 (5580): 400-403. 10.1126/science.1071829.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Bar-Haim Y, Holoshitz Y, Eldar S, Frenkel TI, Muller D, Charney DS, Pine DS, Fox NA, Wald I: Life-threatening danger suppresses attention bias to threat. Am J Psychiatry. 2010, 167: 694-698. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070956.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Scerif G, Karmiloff-Smith A: The dawn of cognitive genetics? Crucial developmental caveats. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005, 9 (3): 126-135. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Bergen SE, Gardner CO, Kendler KS: Age-related changes in heritability of behavioral phenotypes over adolescence and young adulthood: a metaanalysis. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007, 10 (3): 423-433. 10.1375/twin.10.3.423.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Mogg K, Garner M, Bradley BP: Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and fearful faces. Biol Psychol. 2007, 76 (3): 163-169. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.07.005.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Georgiou GA, et al: Focusing on fear: Attentional disengagement from emotional faces. Vis Cogn. 2005, 12 (1): 145-158. 10.1080/13506280444000076.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Bishop SJ: Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nat Neurosci. 2009, 12 (1): 92-98. 10.1038/nn.2242.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Joormann J, Gotlib IH: Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006, 115 (4): 705-714.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Huppert JD, et al: Interpretation biases in social anxiety: response generation, response selection, and self-appraisals. Behav Res Ther. 2007, 45 (7): 1505-1515. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.006.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Lissek S, et al: Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2005, 43 (11): 1391-1424. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Rinck M, et al: Social anxiety predicts avoidance behaviour in virtual encounters. Cognition & Emotion. 2010, 24 (7): 1269-1276. 10.1080/02699930903309268.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Roelofs K, Ly V: Social anxiety and cognitive expectancy of aversive outcome in avoidance conditioning. Behav Res Ther. 2009, 47 (10): 840-847. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.06.015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Mowrer OH: Two-factor learning theory reconsidered, with special reference to secondary reinforcement and the concept of habit. Psychol Rev. 1956, 63 (2): 114-128.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wald I, et al: Battlefield-like stress following simulated combat and suppression of attention bias to threat. Psychol Med. 2011, 41 (4): 699-707. 10.1017/S0033291710002308.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pine DS, et al: Attention bias to threat in maltreated children: implications for vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 2005, 162 (2): 291-296. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Monk CS, et al: Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006, 163 (6): 1091-1097. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.6.1091.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Waters AM, Lipp OV, Spence SH: Attentional bias toward fear-related stimuli: an investigation with nonselected children and adults and children with anxiety disorders. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004, 89 (4): 320-337. 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.06.003.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Waters AM, et al: Attentional bias towards angry faces in childhood anxiety disorders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2010, 41 (2): 158-164. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.12.001.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Waters AM, et al: Threat-based cognitive biases in anxious children: comparison with non-anxious children before and after cognitive behavioural treatment. Behav Res Ther. 2008, 46 (3): 358-374. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Roy AK, et al: Attention bias toward threat in pediatric anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008, 47 (10): 1189-1196. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181825ace.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Vasey MW, et al: Biased attention in childhood anxiety disorders: a preliminary study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995, 23 (2): 267-279. 10.1007/BF01447092.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Dalgleish T, et al: Patterns of processing bias for emotional information across clinical disorders: A comparison of attention, memory, and prospective cognition in children and adolescents with depression, generalized anxiety, and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2003, 32 (1): 10-21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dalgleish T, et al: An experimental investigation of hypervigilance for threat in children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 2001, 31 (3): 541-547.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Monk CS, et al: Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008, 65 (5): 568-576. 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.568.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Keogh E, Hunt C, French CC: Anxiety sensitivity, conscious awareness and selective attentional biases in children. Behav Res Ther. 2007, 45 (3): 497-509. 10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.001.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Heim-Dreger U, et al: Attentional biases for threatening faces in children: Vigilant and avoidant processes. Emotion. 2006, 6 (2): 320-325.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Stirling L, Eley TC, Clark DM: Preliminary evidence for an association between social anxiety symptoms and avoidance of negative faces in school-age children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2006, 35 (3): 440-445.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Vasey MW, ElHag N, Daleiden EL: Anxiety and the processing of emotionally threatening stimuli: Distinctive patterns of selective attention among high- and low-test-anxious children. Child Dev. 1996, 67 (3): 1173-1185. 10.2307/1131886.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Helzer EG, Connor-Smith JK, Reed MA: Traits, states, and attentional gates: Temperament and threat relevance as predictors of attentional bias to social threat. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2009, 22 (1): 57-76. 10.1080/10615800802272244.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Vasey MW, Lonigan CJ: Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control, and Attention to Threat-Relevant Stimuli. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009, 37 (3): 387-399. 10.1007/s10802-008-9284-y.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Telzer EH, et al: Relationship between trait anxiety, prefrontal cortex, and attention bias to angry faces in children and adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2008, 79 (2): 216-222. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.05.004.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Kindt M, Bogels S, Morren M: Processing bias in children with separation anxiety disorder, social phobia and generalised anxiety disorder. Behav Chang. 2003, 20 (3): 143-150. 10.1375/bech.20.3.143.24832.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Kindt M, Brosschot JF, Everaerd W: Cognitive processing bias of children in a real life stress situation and a neutral situation. J Exp Child Psychol. 1997, 64 (1): 79-97. 10.1006/jecp.1996.2336.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF: Cognitive bias in spider fear and control children: Assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a single-trial format of the Stroop task. J Exp Child Psychol. 1997, 66 (2): 163-179. 10.1006/jecp.1997.2376.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Martin M, Jones GV: Integral Bias in the Cognitive Processing of Emotionally Linked Pictures. Br J Psychol. 1995, 86: 419-435. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02761.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Martin M, Horder P, Jones GV: Integral Bias in Naming of Phobia-Related Words. Cognition & Emotion. 1992, 6 (6): 479-486. 10.1080/02699939208409700.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Kindt M, Brosschot JF: Cognitive bias in spider-phobic children: Comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 1999, 21 (3): 207-220. 10.1023/A:1022873331067.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Hadwin JA, et al: Childhood anxiety and attention to emotion faces in a modified stroop task. Br J Dev Psychol. 2009, 27: 487-494. 10.1348/026151008X315503.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Richards A, et al: A comparison of selective attention and facial processing biases in typically developing children who are high and low in self-reported trait anxiety. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19 (2): 481-495.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Richards A, Richards LC, McGeeney A: Anxiety-related stroop interference in adolescents. J Gen Psychol. 2000, 127 (3): 327-333. 10.1080/00221300009598587.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hadwin JA, et al: The influence of children's self-report trait anxiety and depression on visual search for emotional faces. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003, 44 (3): 432-444. 10.1111/1469-7610.00133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Waters AM, Valvoi JS: Attentional bias for emotional faces in paediatric anxiety disorders: An investigation using the emotional go/no go task. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2009, 40 (2): 306-316. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.12.008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ladouceur CD, et al: Processing emotional facial expressions influences performance on a Go/NoGo task in pediatric anxiety and depression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006, 47 (11): 1107-1115. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01640.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Dodd HF, et al: Interpretation bias in preschool children at risk for anxiety: A prospective study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011

    Google Scholar 

  51. Schneider S, et al: Forced choice reaction time paradigm in children with separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, and nonanxious controls. Behav Res Ther. 2009, 47 (12): 1058-1065. 10.1016/j.brat.2009.08.003.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Waters AM, et al: Threat interpretation bias as a vulnerability factor in childhood anxiety disorders. Behav Res Ther. 2008, 46 (1): 39-47. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hughes AA, Kendall PC: Effect of a positive emotional state on interpretation bias for threat in children with anxiety disorders. Emotion. 2008, 8 (3): 414-418.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Barrett PM, et al: Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996, 24 (2): 187-203. 10.1007/BF01441484.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Creswell C, Schniering CA, Rapee RM: Threat interpretation in anxious children and their mothers: comparison with nonclinical children and the effects of treatment. Behav Res Ther. 2005, 43 (10): 1375-1381. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bogels SM, Zigterman D: Dysfunctional cognitions in children with social phobia, separation anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2000, 28 (2): 205-211. 10.1023/A:1005179032470.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Dalgleish T, et al: Information processing in clinically depressed and anxious children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997, 38 (5): 535-541. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01540.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Dineen KA, Hadwin JA: Anxious and depressive symptoms and children's judgements of their own and others' interpretation of ambiguous social scenarios. J Anxiety Disord. 2004, 18 (4): 499-513. 10.1016/S0887-6185(03)00030-6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Eley TC, et al: In the face of uncertainty: A twin study of ambiguous information, anxiety and depression in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008, 36 (1): 55-65. 10.1007/s10802-007-9159-7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Belldolan DJ: Social Cue Interpretation of Anxious Children. J Clin Child Psychol. 1995, 24 (1): 2-10. 10.1207/s15374424jccp2401_1.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Creswell C, O'Connor TG: Interpretation Bias and Anxiety in Childhood: Stability, Specificity and Longitudinal Associations. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2011, 39 (2): 191-204. 10.1017/S1352465810000494.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Muris P, Merckelbach H, Damsma E: Threat perception bias in nonreferred, socially anxious children. J Clin Child Psychol. 2000, 29 (3): 348-359. 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2903_6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Muris P, et al: Threat perception abnormalities in children: the role of anxiety disorders symptoms, chronic anxiety, and state anxiety. J Anxiety Disord. 2003, 17 (3): 271-287. 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00199-8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Bogels SM, Snieder N, Kindt M: Specificity of dysfunctional thinking in children with symptoms of social anxiety, separation anxiety and generalised anxiety. Behav Chang. 2003, 20 (3): 160-169. 10.1375/bech.20.3.160.24836.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Muris P, et al: Threat perception distortions and psychopathological symptoms in typically developing children. Infant Child Dev. 2005, 14 (3): 273-285. 10.1002/icd.392.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Muris P, et al: “Danger is lurking everywhere”. The relation between anxiety and threat perception abnormalities in normal children. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2000, 31 (2): 123-136. 10.1016/S0005-7916(00)00016-1.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Morren M, et al: Emotional Reasoning and Parent-Based Reasoning in Non- Clinical Children, and Their Prospective Relationships with Anxiety Symptoms. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2008, 39 (4): 351-367. 10.1007/s10578-007-0091-4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Muris P, et al: Anxiety and threat perception abnormalities in normal children. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2000, 22 (2): 183-199. 10.1023/A:1007588524525.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Vassilopoulos SP, Banerjee R: Interpretations and judgments regarding positive and negative social scenarios in childhood social anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2008, 46 (7): 870-876. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.03.008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Higa CK, Daleiden EL: Social anxiety and cognitive biases in non-referred children: The interaction of self-focused attention and threat interpretation biases. J Anxiety Disord. 2008, 22 (3): 441-452. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.05.005.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Miers AC, et al: Interpretation bias and social anxiety in adolescents. J Anxiety Disord. 2008, 22 (8): 1462-1471. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.02.010.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Salemink E, Wiers RW: Modifying Threat-related Interpretive Bias in Adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011, 39 (7): 967-976. 10.1007/s10802-011-9523-5.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Taghavi MR, et al: Interpretation of ambiguous emotional information in clinically anxious children and adolescents. Cognition & Emotion. 2000, 14 (6): 809-822. 10.1080/02699930050156645.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Hadwin J, et al: Cognitive processing and trait anxiety in typically developing children: Evidence for an interpretation bias. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997, 106 (3): 486-490.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Simonian SJ, et al: Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents diagnosed with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2001, 32 (2): 137-145. 10.1023/A:1012298707253.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. McClure EB, et al: Facial expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160 (6): 1172-1174. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Beesdo K, et al: Common and Distinct Amygdala-Function Perturbations in Depressed vs Anxious Adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009, 66 (3): 275-285. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.545.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Easter J, et al: Emotion recognition deficits in pediatric anxiety disorders: Implications for amygdala research. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2005, 15 (4): 563-570. 10.1089/cap.2005.15.563.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Craske MG, et al: Is aversive learning a marker of risk for anxiety disorders in children?. Behav Res Ther. 2008, 46 (8): 954-967. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.011.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Lipp OV, et al: Evidence for retarded extinction of aversive learning in anxious children. Behav Res Ther. 2006, 44 (10): 1491-1502. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.11.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Pliszka SR, et al: Classical-Conditioning in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (Adhd) and Anxiety Disorders - a Test of Quays Model. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1993, 21 (4): 411-423. 10.1007/BF01261601.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Waters AM, Henry J, Neumann DL: Aversive Pavlovian Conditioning in Childhood Anxiety Disorders: Impaired Response Inhibition and Resistance to Extinction. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118 (2): 311-321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Lau JYF, et al: Fear conditioning in adolescents with anxiety disorders: Results from a novel experimental paradigm. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008, 47 (1): 94-102. 10.1097/chi.0b01e31815a5f01.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Lau JY, Viding EM: Anxiety-related biases in children's avoidant responses to a masked angry face. Behav Res Ther. 2007, 45 (7): 1639-1645. 10.1016/j.brat.2006.08.006.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Codispoti M, et al: Facial emotion recognition in trait anxiety. J Anxiety Disord. 2006, 20 (1): 110-117. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.010.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Bögels SM, Mansell W: Attention processes in the maintenance and treatment of social phobia: hypervigilance, avoidance and self-focused attention. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004, 24 (7): 827-856. 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.005.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Mathews A, MacLeod C: Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005, 1: 167-195. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Gardner M, Steinberg L: Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev Psychol. 2005, 41 (4): 625-635.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Barlow DH: Anxiety and Its Disorders: The Nature and Treatment of Anxiety and Panic. 1988, Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  90. Krohne HW, Hock M: Relationships Between Restrictive Mother-Child Interactions and Anxiety of the Child. Anxiety Research. 1991, 4: 109-124. 10.1080/08917779108248768.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Rapee RM: The development of generalized anxiety disorder. The developmental psychopathology of anxiety. Edited by: Vasey MW, Dadds MR. 2001, Oxford University Press, New York, 481-504.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Creswell C, Schniering CA, Rapee RM: Threat interpretation in anxious children and their mothers: comparison with nonclinical children and the effects of treatment. Behavior Research and Therapy. 2005, 43: 1375-1381. 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.009.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Barrett DE: The Three Stages of Adolescence. High Sch J. 1996, 79 (4): 333-339.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Hadwin JA, Garner M, Perez-Olivas G: The development of information processing biases in childhood anxiety: a review and exploration of its origins in parenting. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006, 26 (7): 876-894. 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.09.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Lau JYF, et al: Individual Differences in Children's Facial Expression Recognition Ability: The Role of Nature and Nurture. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009, 34 (1): 37-51. 10.1080/87565640802564424.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Plomin R, Trouton A, Spinath FM: Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A multivariate, longitudinal genetic investigation of language, cognition and behavior problems in childhood. Twin Res. 2002, 5 (5): 444-448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Gregory AM, et al: Genetic and environmental influences on interpersonal cognitions and associations with depressive symptoms in 8-year-old twins. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007, 116 (4): 762-775.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Lau JY, et al: Assessing gene-environment interactions on anxiety symptom subtypes across childhood and adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2007, 19 (4): 1129-1146.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Goodman R, et al: The Development and Well-Being Assessment: Description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000, 41 (5): 645-655. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2000.tb02345.x.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Fombonne E, et al: Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders in the British nationwide survey of child mental health. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001, 40 (7): 820-827. 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00017.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Birmaher B, et al: The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997, 36 (4): 545-553. 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Kessels RPC, et al: The emotion recognition task: A paradigm to measure the perception of facial emotional expressions at different intensities. Percept Mot Skills. 2007, 104 (2): 589-598. 10.2466/pms.104.2.589-598.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Gagliardi C, et al: Facial expression recognition in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41 (6): 733-738. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00178-1.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Frigerio E, et al: Facial affect perception in alcoholics. Psychiatry Res. 2002, 113 (1–2): 161-171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Mowrer OH: On the dual nature of learning-a re-interpretation of "conditioning" and "problem-solving". Harv Educ Rev. 1947, 17: 102-148.

    Google Scholar 

  106. Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ: Conscious and Unconscious Emotional Learning in the Human Amygdala. Nature. 1998, 393: 467-470. 10.1038/30976.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Elkman P: Freisen WV: Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. 1978, Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto, California

    Google Scholar 

  108. Neale MC, et al: Mx: Statistical Modeling. 1999, Department of Psychiatry, VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298, 5

    Google Scholar 

  109. Felsenfeld S, et al: A Study of the Genetic and Enviromental Etiology of Stuttering in a Selected Twin Sample. Behav Genet. 2000, 30: 359-366. 10.1023/A:1002765620208.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Simonoff E, et al: The Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent behavioral development - Influences of age, sex, and impairment on rates of disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997, 54 (9): 801-808. 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830210039004.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Merikangas KR, et al: Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication--Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010, 49 (10): 980-989. 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Eley TC, et al: A Twin Study of Anxiety-Related Behaviours in Pre-school Children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003, 44 (7): 945-960. 10.1111/1469-7610.00179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Gregory AM, et al: Juvenile Mental Health Histories of Adults with Anxiety Disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2007, 164 (2): 301-308. 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.301.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Muris P, et al: Disgust sensitivity, trait anxiety and anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Behav Res Ther. 1999, 37 (10): 953-961. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00045-5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Amir N, et al: Increased activation of the anterior cingulate cortex during processing of disgust faces in individuals with social phobia. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 57 (9): 975-981. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.044.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all participants from the ECHO study. We are also grateful to Jeanette Augustin, James Blair, Pippa Carter, David Clark, Orla Jordan, Helen Mathews, Peter McGuffin, Maria Napolitano, Robert Plomin and Nina Shiffrin for their assistance on various aspects of the study. We would also like to acknowledge Michael Burt who developed and programmed the task on facial expression recognition. Data collection for this study was supported by a Career Development Award from the UK MRC to TE. JL was supported by a MRC Studentship during the data collection phase of this research. AG is currently supported by a Leverhulme Trust Fellowship.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer Y F Lau.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JL, AG and TE conceived of the study design and carried out the testing of participants. EV and DP contributed to the design of the experimental tasks used. JL conducted the statistical analysis. RG rated all diagnostic data from the DAWBA. JL and KH conducted the brief review of studies in the article and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lau, J.Y.F., Hilbert, K., Goodman, R. et al. Investigating the genetic and environmental bases of biases in threat recognition and avoidance in children with anxiety problems. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2, 12 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-2-12

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-2-12

Keywords